This is a Circuit Judge decision about whether a vulnerable witness (the children’s aunt) should give evidence in a way that would prevent the father (about whom she was making very serious allegations of abuse when she was a child) from seeing her. By either screens blocking his view, or by video link.
The father argued that this would not be fair
5.The father’s position turns on the relevance of demeanour in assessing the credibility of a witness. The father says that the court’s ability to assess the credibility of the witness will be compromised if the court is unable to assess the witness’s demeanour when giving evidence. The father goes further to say that it is necessary that he should be able to assess the witness’s demeanour when she is giving evidence.
The Judge, His Honour Judge Greensmith, gave a judgment, and set out the history of judicial views of demeanour and the current position. (Drawn from the Court of Appeal decision this year in an immigration case called Singh)
The history began with Judges thinking that the demeanour of a witness was vital in deciding whether they were telling the truth, and now is very cautious about the value of demeanour in deciding this. It’s an excellent summary of the law, and is, to my mind, very interesting about the shift in views.
The whole issue of how good people are at detecting when another person is lying is fascinating. Every study since 1986 has revealed that people are essentially no better in deciding whether a person is lying or telling the truth than pure luck – if you decided not to try to determine it by skill and just flipped a coin and said ‘head they’re honest, tails they’re a liar’ most people would get better results than using all of their finely honed instincts. Most of the behavioural markers we think indicate dishonesty simply aren’t reliable. And indeed, there’s some empirical evidence that using impressions of demeanour (tone of voice, position of eyes, body language) makes the assessor even worse at establishing whether someone is telling the truth than if they paid such things no regard at all and just decided honesty based on the content of what was said.
Professional lie-catchers (such as police officers and customs officers) have been found in studies to be no better at determining whether an individual is being honest or telling the truth than a random member of the public (Vrij 2000)
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/248925175_How_people_really_detect_lies
http://www.bailii.org/ew/cases/EWFC/OJ/2018/B69.html
M & Ors v Liverpool City Council [2018] EWFC B69 (12 November 2018)
6.The relevance of demeanour as an indicator of credibility is questionable. It is the current teaching of the Judicial College during courses on “judge-craft” to the judiciary across all jurisdictions, including newly appointed Deputy High Court Judges, that judges should be very circumspect about the value of demeanour in assessing credibility.
7.This principle is emphasised in the case which I brought the parties’ attention to which is Sri Lanka v. the Secretary of State for the Home Department [2018] England and Wales Court of Appeal Civ 1391 and in particular paragraphs 33 onwards. I set this out on full to inform how the current approach to demeanour is developed by Leggatt LJ:
Demeanour
- The term “demeanour” is used as a legal shorthand to refer to the appearance and behaviour of a witness in giving oral evidence as opposed to the content of the evidence. The concept is, in the words of Lord Shaw in Clarke v Edinburgh & District Tramways Co Ltd 1919 SC (HL) 35, 36, that:
“witnesses … may have in their demeanour, in their manner, in their hesitation, in the nuance of their expressions, in even the turns of the eyelid, left an impression upon the man who saw and heard them which can never be reproduced in the printed page.”
- The opportunity of a trial judge or other finder of fact to observe the demeanour of witnesses when they testify and to take this into account in assessing the credibility of their testimony used to be regarded as a peculiar advantage over an appellate court which insulated findings of fact based on such observation from challenge on appeal. This approach was encapsulated by Lord Sumner in Owners of Steamship Hontestroom v Owners of Steamship Sagaporack [1947] AC 37, 47, when he said that:
“… not to have seen the witnesses puts appellate judges in a permanent position of disadvantage as against the trial judge, and, unless it can be shown that he has failed to use or has palpably misused his advantage, the higher Court ought not to take the responsibility of reversing conclusions so arrived at, merely on the result of their own comparisons and criticisms of the witnesses and of their own view of the probabilities of the case.”
- Nowadays the reluctance of an appellate court to interfere with findings of fact made after a trial or similar hearing is generally justified on other grounds: in particular, the greater opportunity afforded to the first instance court or tribunal to absorb the detail and nuances of the evidence, considerations of cost and the efficient use of judicial resources and the expectation of the parties that, as Lewison LJ put it in Fage UK Ltd v Chobani UK Ltd [2014] EWCA Civ 5, para 114(ii):
“The trial is not a dress rehearsal. It is the first and last night of the show.”
- Generally speaking, it is no longer considered that inability to assess the demeanour of witnesses puts appellate judges “in a permanent position of disadvantage as against the trial judge”. That is because it has increasingly been recognised that it is usually unreliable and often dangerous to draw a conclusion from a witness’s demeanour as to the likelihood that the witness is telling the truth. The reasons for this were explained by MacKenna J in words which Lord Devlin later adopted in their entirety and Lord Bingham quoted with approval:
“I question whether the respect given to our findings of fact based on the demeanour of the witnesses is always deserved. I doubt my own ability, and sometimes that of other judges, to discern from a witness’s demeanour, or the tone of his voice, whether he is telling the truth. He speaks hesitantly. Is that the mark of a cautious man, whose statements are for that reason to be respected, or is he taking time to fabricate? Is the emphatic witness putting on an act to deceive me, or is he speaking from the fullness of his heart, knowing that he is right? Is he likely to be more truthful if he looks me straight in the face than if he casts his eyes on the ground perhaps from shyness or a natural timidity? For my part I rely on these considerations as little as I can help.”
“Discretion” (1973) 9 Irish Jurist (New Series) 1, 10, quoted in Devlin, The Judge (1979) p63 and Bingham, “The Judge as Juror: The Judicial Determination of Factual Issues” (1985) 38 Current Legal Problems 1 (reprinted in Bingham, The Business of Judging p9).
- The reasons for distrusting reliance on demeanour are magnified where the witness is of a different nationality from the judge and is either speaking English as a foreign language or is giving evidence through an interpreter. Scrutton LJ once said that he had “never yet seen a witness giving evidence through an interpreter as to whom I could decide whether he was telling the truth or not”: see Compania Naviera Martiartu v Royal Exchange Assurance Corp (1922) 13 Ll L Rep 83, 97. In his seminal essay on “The Judge as Juror” Lord Bingham observed:
“If a Turk shows signs of anger when accused of lying, is that to be interpreted as the bluster of a man caught out in deceit or the reaction of an honest man to an insult? If a Greek, similarly challenged, becomes rhetorical and voluble and offers to swear the truth of what he has said on the lives of his children, what (if any) significance should be attached to that? If a Japanese witness, accused of forging a document, becomes sullen, resentful and hostile, does this suggest that he has done so or that he has not? I can only ask these questions. I cannot answer them. And if the answer is given that it all depends on the impression made by the particular witness in the particular case that is in my view no answer. The enigma usually remains. To rely on demeanour is in most cases to attach importance to deviations from a norm when there is in truth no norm.” (emphasis added)
See Bingham, “The Judge as Juror: The Judicial Determination of Factual Issues” (1985) 38 Current Legal Problems 1 (reprinted in Bingham, The Business of Judging at p11).
- Ms Jegarajah emphasised that immigration judges acquire considerable experience of observing persons of different nationalities and ethnicities giving oral evidence and suggested that this makes those judges expert in evaluating the credibility of testimony given by such persons based on their demeanour. I have no doubt that immigration judges do learn much in the course of their work about different cultural attitudes and customs and that such knowledge can help to inform their decision-making in beneficial ways. But it would hubristic for any judge to suppose that because he or she has, for example, seen a number of individuals of Tamil origin giving oral evidence this gives him or her a privileged insight into whether a particular witness of that ethnicity is telling the truth. That would be to assume that there are typical characteristics shared by members of an ethnic group (or by human beings generally) which can be relied on to differentiate a person who is lying from someone who is telling what they believe to be the truth. I know of no evidence to suggest that any such characteristics exist or that demeanour provides any reliable indication of how likely it is that a witness is giving honest testimony.
- To the contrary, empirical studies confirm that the distinguished judges from whom I have quoted were right to distrust inferences based on demeanour. The consistent findings of psychological research have been summarised in an American law journal as follows:
“Psychologists and other students of human communication have investigated many aspects of deceptive behavior and its detection. As part of this investigation, they have attempted to determine experimentally whether ordinary people can effectively use nonverbal indicia to determine whether another person is lying. In effect, social scientists have tested the legal premise concerning demeanor as a scientific hypothesis. With impressive consistency, the experimental results indicate that this legal premise is erroneous. According to the empirical evidence, ordinary people cannot make effective use of demeanor in deciding whether to believe a witness. On the contrary, there is some evidence that the observation of demeanor diminishes rather than enhances the accuracy of credibility judgments.”
OG Wellborn, “Demeanor” (1991) 76 Cornell LR 1075. See further Law Commission Report No 245 (1997) “Evidence in Criminal Proceedings”, paras 3.9–3.12. While the studies mentioned involved ordinary people, there is no reason to suppose that judges have any extraordinary power of perception which other people lack in this respect.
- This is not to say that judges (or jurors) lack the ability to tell whether witnesses are lying. Still less does it follow that there is no value in oral evidence. But research confirms that people do not in fact generally rely on demeanour to detect deception but on the fact that liars are more likely to tell stories that are illogical, implausible, internally inconsistent and contain fewer details than persons telling the truth: see Minzner, “Detecting Lies Using Demeanor, Bias and Context” (2008) 29 Cardozo LR 2557. One of the main potential benefits of cross-examination is that skilful questioning can expose inconsistencies in false stories.
41 No doubt it is impossible, and perhaps undesirable, to ignore altogether the impression created by the demeanour of a witness giving evidence. But to attach any significant weight to such impressions in assessing credibility risks making judgments which at best have no rational basis and at worst reflect conscious or unconscious biases and prejudices. One of the most important qualities expected of a judge is that they will strive to avoid being influenced by personal biases and prejudices in their decision-making. That requires eschewing judgments based on the appearance of a witness or on their tone, manner or other aspects of their behaviour in answering questions. Rather than attempting to assess whether testimony is truthful from the manner in which it is given, the only objective and reliable approach is to focus on the content of the testimony and to consider whether it is consistent with other evidence (including evidence of what the witness has said on other occasions) and with known or probable facts.
- This was the approach which the FTT judge adopted in the present case. It appears that the FTT judge did in fact recall when writing the determination the manner in which the appellant gave evidence at the hearing, as he commented (at para 59):
“When [the appellant] gave evidence before me, some of his answers were inconsistent and variable but there was no suggestion that he could not remember things.”
This suggests that the way in which the appellant answered questions did not create a favourable impression. Quite rightly, however, the FTT judge did not attach weight to that impression in assessing the credibility of the appellant’s account. Instead, he focussed on whether the facts alleged by the appellant were plausible, consistent with objectively verifiable information and consistent with what the appellant had said on other occasions (in particular, at his asylum interview and in recounting his history to the medical experts). Applying those standards, the FTT judge found numerous significant inconsistencies and improbable features in the appellant’s account which he set out in detail in the determination. As the FTT judge explained, it was “the cumulative effect of the implausible and inconsistent evidence” given by the appellant which led him to conclude that the core of the appellant’s account was not credible.
- Accordingly, even if the appellant had through his demeanour when answering questions given the FTT judge the impression that he looked and sounded believable, the suggestion that the FTT judge should have given significant weight to that impression, let alone that he could properly have treated it as compensating for the many inconsistencies and improbabilities in the content of the appellant’s account, cannot be accepted.
- Applying the approach of the Court of Appeal, I am compelled to make it clear to the parties that I am and will be very circumspect about the reliability of demeanour of any witness in these proceedings, including the father’s sister. It follows that if the court takes that approach to demeanour, so should the father in his assessment of the credibility of the sister.
Very useful summary for anyone in a case where these issues arise. Or for practitioners generally. Many of us like to think that we are brilliant at gauging whether someone is lying, but the reality is that we’re nowhere near as good as we think and the best of us are slightly better than chance. It is the content and the correlation of the content with the other evidence that lets us decide on veracity, not whether someone makes eye contact or rubs their nose, or speaks confidently.
In the case in question, the Judge decided that the witness could give evidence by video link.
(In case you are pondering the title, Miss Demeanour was a Viz character, in the Beryl the Peril mode, who had a series of (chiefly violent) adventures with her concertina)
Misses the point completely in my opinion.In cross-examination it is more important to see if a question worries or distresses the witness than whether or not they are being truthful.
If a question threatens to force the revealing of facts that the witness seeks to conceal the dismay of the witness will encourage more questions on the same lines.The demeanour of the witness can give away the path the questioner should tread and the path the witness may hope to avoid.
Screens should be avoided in all cases including those involving rape victims and children in fairness to the person accused who in some cases may be 100% innocent.
I can’t see how it’s a guessing game at all, profilers use body language as one of the first ports of call.
I can’t agree with no screening for anyone, vulnerable victims are terrified of their abusers and certainly won’t testify while feeling intimidated face to face. Especially if they’re facing a narcissistic personality, who will quite convincingly lie their way through just about any situation
I NEVER said victims should be questioned by those accused of abusing them ! I said NO SCREENS and of course the lawyer represting the accused should do the questioning and if none then the court should appoint one for that purpose as indeed they often do already to stop parents cross examining their own children !
I didn’t mention victims being questioned Ian. Just being in the same room – seeing them is enough to put them off
Going back to 1215 and Magna Carta we have always had the right to “face our accusers” but not necessarily the right to question them in person .